Donate | Join/Renew | Print Page | Contact Us | Report Abuse | Sign In
Behavioral Science Response to COVID-19 Working Group
Share |

Behavioral Science Response to COVID-19 Working Group

 

COVID-19 and Behavioral Science
The spread of COVID-19 is, in part, based on human behavior. Behavioral scientists are a unique resource for changing human behavior in ways that may reduce the spread of the virus. The Psychonomic Society initiated an effort to assemble a group of experts in learning and behavior modification. Our goal is to disseminate evidence-based recommendations in areas where behavioral science may make a unique contribution.


Resources to Slow the Spread of COVID-19

1. How to Reduce Face Touching

Our infographic on How to Reduce Face Touching is the first resource produced by the Working Group. The infographic, as seen below, is now available in 20+ languages. View, download, and share!

Download the PDF
(PDF available in 20+ languages)

2. Practical Tips for Social Distancing

Over time, stay-at-home orders are likely to be gradually lifted in many localities. In this context, it is more important than ever that we strengthen our ability to successfully social distance. The following infographic provides practical information on why social distancing matters and how to practice conscientious social distancing in public environments. View, download, and share!

Download the PDF
(PDF available in 20+ languages)

3. Hand Washing

The novel coronavirus spreads through human interactions with people who are infected. Therefore, changing human behavior is a powerful, low cost, immediate intervention to stem the pandemic. Our latest infographic provides evidence-based recommendations to promote hand washing. View, download, and share!

Download the PDF
(New translations coming soon)


About the Behavioral Science Response to COVID-19 Working Group

The Psychonomic Society is launching new initiatives to respond to COVID-19. The first was originally directed toward reducing the carbon footprint of the society, in part by adapting our annual meeting to a hybrid or even purely virtual format, thereby curtailing carbon-intensive airplane travel. Now, the rapid rise of the pandemic makes a virtual conference even more pressing. Remotely accessible conferences offer further advantages by opening up our meeting to people who live far away from the meeting venue or who lack the means or the time to travel because of family or other obligations. Virtual meetings are also far more inclusive for those with disabilities, for whom physical attendance is especially challenging.

This latest initiative capitalizes on the extensive expertise in behavioral science within our membership. The coronavirus spreads through human behavior, and so it can best be contained if we teach people best practices for how to change critical behaviors such as their hand washing, social distancing, and in this instance, face touching. Research has shown that we touch our faces far more often than we may realize, about 23 times per hour, and this creates a major path for the spread of the infection.

We live in a world filled with misinformation, myths, and misguided advice, often offered with good intentions but limited knowledge about the science of pandemics. The recommendations we give here on how to reduce face touching is based on what we know to be true from published studies, or in some cases on what we believe is extremely likely to be true, based on findings regarding similar behaviors. With the help of our colleagues, we hope to extend this first effort with further messages addressing other behaviors, such as hand washing and social distancing, that play a key role in the transmission of COVID-19.

 
References (How to Reduce Face Touching)
Our evidence-based recommendations to reduce face touching are based on the following references:

  1. Ellingson, S. A., Miltenberger, R. G., Stricker, J. M., Garlinghouse, M. A., Roberts, J., Galensky, T. L., & Rapp, J. T. (2000). Analysis and treatment of finger sucking. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33(1), 41-52. http://doi: 10.1901/jaba.2000.33-41
  2. Ghanizadeh, A., Bazrafshan, A., Firoozabadi, A., & Dehbozorgi, G. (2013). Habit Reversal versus Object Manipulation Training for Treating Nail Biting: A Randomized Controlled Clinical Trial. Iranian journal of psychiatry, 8(2), 61-67. PMCID: PMC3796295
  3. Long, E. S., Miltenberger, R. G., Ellingson, S. A., & Ott, S. M. (1999). Augmenting simplified habit reversal in the treatment of oral-digital habits exhibited by individuals with mental retardation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(3), 353-365. http://doi: 10.1901/jaba.1999.32-353
  4. Miltenberger, R. G., Fuqua, R. W., & Woods, D. W. (1998). Applying behavior analysis to clinical problems: review and analysis of habit reversaL. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31(3), 447-469. http://doi: 10.1901/jaba.1998.31-447
  5. Rapp, J. T., Miltenberger, R. G., & Long, E. S. (1998). Augmenting simplified habit reversal with an awareness enhancement device: preliminary findings. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31(4), 665-668. http://doi: 10.1901/jaba.1998.31-665
  6. Stricker, J. M., Miltenberger, R. G., Garlinghouse, M. A., Deaver, C. M., & Anderson, C. A. (2001). Evaluation of an awareness enhancement device for the treatment of thumb sucking in children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34(1), 77-80. http://doi: 10.1901/jaba.2001.34-77
  7. Twohig, M. P., & Woods, D. W. (2001). Evaluating the duration of the competing response in habit reversal: a parametric analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34(4), 517-520. http://doi: 10.1901/jaba.2001.34-517
  8. Woods, D. W., & Miltenberger, R. G. (1995). Habit reversal: A review of applications and variations. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 26(2), 123-131. http://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(95)00009-O
  9. Woods, D. W., Murray, L. K., Fuqua, R. W., Seif, T. A., Boyer, L. J., & Siah, A. (1999). Comparing the effectiveness of similar and dissimilar competing responses in evaluating the habit reversal treatment for oral–digital habits in children. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 30(4), 289-300. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7916(99)00031-2

Additional sources of information:
https://www.apa.org/practice/programs/dmhi/research-information/pandemics


References (Practical Tips on Social Distancing)

The following are many of the references to the scientific papers that informed our recommendations on social distancing:

  1. Atkinson, J., Chartier, Y., Pessoa-Silva, C.L., Jensen, P., Li, Y., Seto, W.-H. (Eds). (2009). Natural Ventilation for Infection Control in Health-Care Settings. World Health Organization, Geneva, Switzerland. ISBN 978 92 4 154785 7 (NLM classification:WX 167)

  2. Block, P., Hoffman, M., Raabe, I. J., Dowd, J. B., Rahal, C., Kashyap, R., & Mills, M. C. (2020). Social network-based distancing strategies to flatten the COVID 19 curve in a post-lockdown world. arXiv preprint arXiv:2004.07052.

  3. Cialdini, R.B. (2001). Harnessing the Science of Persuasion. Harvard Business Review, October 2001, Reprint r0109d.

  4. Cialdini, R.B. (2006). Influence: The Psychology of Persuasion, Revised Edition. Harper Business.

  5. Cialdini, R. B. (2020). Reducing Undesirable COVID-19 Behaviors. https://www.influenceatwork.com/inside-influence-report/advice-for-reducing-undesirable-covid-19-behaviors/

  6. Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: strong effects of simple plans. American psychologist, 54(7), 493. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.493

  7. Linkenauger, S. A., Bulthoff, H. H., & Mohler, B. J. (2015). Virtual arm’s reach influences perceived distance but only after experience reaching. Neuropsychologia, 70, 393–401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.034.

  8. Linkenauger, S. A., Leyrer, M., Bülthoff, H. H., & Mohler, B. J. (2013). Welcome to wonderland: the influence of the size and shape of a virtual hand on the perceived size and shape of virtual objects. PloS one, 8(7), e68594. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068594

  9. Liu, L. L., & Park, D. C. (2004). Aging and medical adherence: the use of automatic processes to achieve effortful things. Psychology and aging, 19(2), 318. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.318

  10. Locey, M. L., & Rachlin, H. (2015). Altruism and anonymity: A behavioral analysis. Behavioural Processes, 118, 71–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.06.002

  11. Jones, B. A., & Rachlin, H. (2009). Delay, probability, and social discounting in a public goods game. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 91(1), 61–73. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2009.91-61

  12. Martin S. J., Goldstein, N., & Cialdini, R. B. (2014), The small BIG: small changes that spark big influence. Grand Central Publishing.

  13. McFarland, C., & Glisky, E. (2012). Implementation intentions and imagery: Individual and combined effects on prospective memory among young adults. Memory & cognition, 40(1), 62-69. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-011-0126-8

  14. Parshina-Kottas, Y., Saget, B., Patanjali, K., Fleisher, O., Gianordoli, G. (2020). This 3-D simulation shows why social distancing is so important. The New York Times, April 14. https://www.nytimes.com/interactive/2020/04/14/science/coronavirus-transmission-cough-6-feet-ar-ul.html

  15. Proffitt, D. R. & Linkenauger, S. A. (2013). Perception viewed as a phenotypic expression. In: Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline, ed. W. Prinz, M. Beisert & A. Herwig, pp. 171–98. MIT Press.

  16. Stevens, H. (2020).  Why outbreaks like coronavirus spread exponentially, and how to “flatten the curve”. Washington Post, March. https://www.washingtonpost.com/graphics/2020/world/corona-simulator/

  17. Yang, S., Lee, G.W.M., Chen, C.-M., Wu, C.-C., Yu, K.-P. (2007). The size and concentration of droplets generated by coughing in human subjects. Journal of Aerosol Medicine, 20(4), 484-494.

  18. Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D.R., & Epstein, W. (2005).  Tool use affects perceived distance but only when you intend to use it.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 880-888.

  19. World NHK-Report, SARS-CoV-2 Coronavirus Micro-droplets, https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=vBvFkQizTT4

Additional sources of information:
American Psychological Association


Questions?

Contact Member Services at info@psychonomic.org or +1 847-375-3696.



COVID-19
Working Group


Jonathon Crystal
Indiana University Bloomington, USA
(Chair)

James Pomerantz
Rice University, USA
(Co-Chair)

Kate Bruce
University of North Carolina Wilmington, USA

Andy Delamater
Brooklyn College, USA

Claudia Dozier
University of Kansas, USA

Wayne Fuqua
Western Michigan University, USA

Mark Galizio
University of North Carolina Wilmington, USA

Rachel Jess
University of Kansas, USA

Megan Heinicke
Sacramento State University, USA

Debbie Kelly
University of Manitoba, Canada

Sachiko Koyama
Indiana University Bloomington, USA

Olga Lazareva
Drake University, USA

Linda LeBlanc
LeBlanc Behavioral Consulting, USA

Mark McDaniel
Washington University in St. Louis, USA

Laura Mickes
University of Bristol, UK

Raymond Miltenberger
University of South Florida, USA

Matthew Normand
University of the Pacific, USA

Amy Odum
Utah State University, USA

Danielle Panoz-Brown
Indiana University Bloomington, USA 

Gordon Pennycook
University of Regina, Canada

Penny Pexman
University of Calgary, Canada

Suparna Rajaram
Stony Brook University, USA

Phil Reed
Swansea University, UK

Crystal Slanzi
University of Florida, USA

Carole Van Camp
University of North Carolina Wilmington, USA

Tim Vollmer
University of Florida, USA

Wendy Donlin Washington
University of North Carolina Wilmington, USA

Jessica Witt
Colorado State University, USA

Doug Woods
Marquette University, USA



  8735 W. Higgins Road, Suite 300 • Chicago, IL 60631 USA
Phone: +1 847-375-3696 • Fax: +1 847-375-6449 • Email: info@psychonomic.org

Use of Articles
Legal Notice

Privacy Policy